Bringing attention into higher focus within the traumatic brain injury research agenda
Editorial

Bringing attention into higher focus within the traumatic brain injury research agenda

Christopher M. Horvat, Michael J. Bell

Department of Critical Care Medicine and Safar Center for Resuscitation Research, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, USA

Correspondence to: Michael J. Bell. Faculty Pavillion, Children’s Hospital of Pittsburgh, 400 45th Street, 2nd Floor, Pittsburgh, PA 15201, USA. Email: bellmj4@upmc.edu.

Submitted Oct 09, 2015. Accepted for publication Oct 13, 2015.

doi: 10.3978/j.issn.2224-4336.2015.10.05


Traumatic brain injury (TBI) holds a unique position within children’s health. It has been clear for decades that TBI is the leading cause of death and disability of children (1,2). However, only recently has the impact of TBI on developing brain gained the attention of the public and lay press due to the attention paid toward injuries in sports such as football, boxing and others. Guidelines for caring for children with mild (3-5) and severe (6) injuries have been assiduously developed from the available literature, yet the proven therapies have remained elusive. Recently, the National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS), the European Commission and the Canadian Institutes of Health Research have led (and funded) efforts to address the burden of TBI with the International Initiative for Traumatic Brain Injury Research (InTBIR) with the goal of “working together to improve outcomes and lessen the global burden of TBI by 2020” (7).

To achieve such lofty goals, an honest assessment regarding the state of the field is required. Overall, the field has been hampered by the failure of large interventional studies in both adults and children, with several theories having been offered over to explain these circumstances. In the IMPACT study by Drs. Maas and colleagues, the study team combined already-completed studies into a common database to overcome difficulties with statistical power in an attempt to uncover ways to improve clinical care. In this database of almost 10,000 subjects, they found that inter-center variation was large and the main predictor of patient outcome (8,9). In another effort, Saatman and colleagues argued that neuroimaging findings—such as epidural and subdural hematomas, focal contusions, diffuse injuries—can play important roles in presenting symptoms and patient outcomes (10). They theorized that the current TBI classification system based solely on the Glasgow Coma Scale (GCS) score is inadequate to fully categorize patients and this mis-classification leads to failed studies. In pediatrics, we have found that an international consortium of pediatric neurotrauma centers have very diverse goals for their medical therapies, including differences related to metabolic support, intracranial hypertension therapies and secondary insults (11). These preliminary efforts have led to three large international studies of TBI—Collaborative European NeuroTrauma Effectiveness Research in Traumatic Brain Injury (CENTER-TBI), Transforming Research and Clinical Knowledge in Traumatic Brain Injury (TRACK-TBI) and the Approaches and Decisions for Acute Pediatric TBI trial (ADAPT Trial)—that are addressing these concerns.

Another important theory regarding the failure of previous studies revolves around selecting the appropriate outcome test for a given population. Over the past decades, it has become recognized that residual sequelae of TBI can range from subtle alterations in cognition to profound psychomotor disability. In addition to the clear deficits rendered by more severe brain trauma, injuries once considered inconsequential are now appreciated to have longer-lasting effects. While ostensibly severe consequences of brain injury such as the inability to perform self-care are obviously associated with monumental individual and societal burdens, more common—though less apparent—changes in function following TBI may have a larger role in the population-level disease burden. The high incidence of mild TBI, coupled with such sequelae, has been demonstrated to confer a significantly larger population-level burden of disability compared to moderate or severe TBI in children (12).

The lack of standard outcome measures across TBI studies has recently begun to be addressed by the TBI community. NINDS organized several expert panels to identify common data elements of TBI studies, with the desire that eventually combining studies may be revealing (13-16). As a part of this process, a group of outcome experts were empaneled to identify tests from relevant neuropsychological domains that should be performed on children with TBI (17). The group outlined 14 distinct domains, including global outcomes, academics, adaptive living skills, quality of life, physical functioning and TBI-related symptoms, and then identified core tests that should be included within each domain. This has been operationalized by NINDS on their website (https://commondataelements.ninds.nih.gov/tbi.aspx) for ongoing use.

Within these domains, the question of attention is included in the TBI-related symptoms domain along with memory difficulties and emotional difficulties. In the most broad sense, deficits in attention are common after TBI (18), but it is less clear which aspects of attention are affected or the optimal methods for investigating the issue (19,20). In this issue of the journal, Königs and colleagues work to distinguish the nature of attentional problems in children who have experienced TBI (21). This retrospective case-control study enrolled children ages 6 to 13 years diagnosed with TBI (case) or non-TBI trauma (control). Participants were assessed with the Attention Network Test (ANT), which the authors note holds an advantage of being able to measure the efficiency of alerting, orienting and executive attention while minimizing the confounding inherent to paper and pencil tests, which are influenced by participants’ visuomotor limitations. Participants were stratified according to the contemporary TBI severity designations of mild, moderate and severe, with the mild group being additionally stratified according to whether they demonstrated risk factors for complicated TBI (22). Parents and teachers of children who completed the ANT were asked to fill out the child behavior checklist, strength and difficulties questionnaire. Ex-Gaussian analysis was used to distinguish the contribution of extremely slow responses to information processing speed and to adjust for the impact of extremely slow responses when determining information processing speed.

In aggregate, children with TBI had lower full scale IQ, had a longer mean reaction time while completing the ANT, and experienced longer lapses in attention. The relative difference in performance between alerting, orienting and executive attention tasks were preserved between the control and TBI patients, indicating that TBI patients had no specific deficits in any of these domains relative to non-brain-injured controls. Further, parents of children with TBI were more likely to note attention problems, as well as internalizing and externalizing problems, in their children when compared to parents of the children in the control group. Interestingly, a mediation model demonstrated that lapses in attention accounted for the relation between measured intelligence and parents’ perceptions of attention problems. When categories of TBI severity were examined, it was noted that risk-factor negative mild TBI was not associated with perceived or measured deficits in attention.

The study is intriguing in its central finding that lapses in attention, rather than deficits in a particular attention domain, are the main drivers for disparate reaction times between TBI patients and non-TBI controls. The authors contrast their findings with a recent review by Ginstfelt and Emanuelson that highlighted impairments in executive attention following TBI (20). That same review, however, noted that attention span was the most vulnerable domain following traumatic injury, particularly in the first year following injury and beyond one year for those with severe injury.

TBI deficits evolve, if not improve, over time. An important limitation of the current study is the lack of longitudinal follow-up. The mean timespan between the injury and study participation was 1.6 and 1.7 years in controls and TBI patients, respectively. Patterns of post-injury recovery in pediatrics are likely both age and temporally-dependent given the plasticity of the pediatric brain. As previously mentioned, some evidence suggests that the first post-injury year is an important timeframe when assessing TBI-related attention deficits. Whether the same magnitude of difference between the control and TBI population in this study holds as time from injury increases would be interesting to examine. Another obvious limitation to the ANT instrument is its requirement to be performed in Dutch. However, in this way, the test is common with other instruments that have only been validated in selected languages, usually English.

Königs and colleagues have helped clarify the characteristics of attention deficits following pediatric TBI. Their study, along with studies conducted by others that examine how children recover from TBI, must be incorporated into the ongoing work of investigators around the world. Examining whether these findings remain intact longitudinally will be important for guiding therapies and treatments aimed at improving attention in these patients.


Acknowledgements

None.


Footnote

Conflicts of Interest: The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.


References

  1. Coronado VG, Xu L, Basavaraju SV, et al. Surveillance for traumatic brain injury-related deaths--United States, 1997-2007. MMWR Surveill Summ 2011;60:1-32. [PubMed]
  2. Faul M, Coronado V. Epidemiology of traumatic brain injury. Handb Clin Neurol 2015;127:3-13. [PubMed]
  3. Kamerling SN, Lutz N, Posner JC, et al. Mild traumatic brain injury in children: practice guidelines for emergency department and hospitalized patients. The Trauma Program, The Children's Hospital of Philadelphia, University of Pennsylvania School of Medicine. Pediatr Emerg Care 2003;19:431-40. [PubMed]
  4. Marshall S, Bayley M, McCullagh S, et al. Clinical practice guidelines for mild traumatic brain injury and persistent symptoms. Can Fam Physician 2012;58:257-67, e128-40.
  5. Marshall S, Bayley M, McCullagh S, et al. Updated clinical practice guidelines for concussion/mild traumatic brain injury and persistent symptoms. Brain Inj 2015;29:688-700. [PubMed]
  6. Kochanek PM, Carney N, Adelson PD, et al. Guidelines for the acute medical management of severe traumatic brain injury in infants, children, and adolescents--second edition. Pediatr Crit Care Med 2012;13 Suppl 1:S1-82.
  7. Tosetti P, Hicks RR, Theriault E, et al. Toward an international initiative for traumatic brain injury research. J Neurotrauma 2013;30:1211-22. [PubMed]
  8. Lingsma HF, Roozenbeek B, Li B, Lu J, et al. Large between-center differences in outcome after moderate and severe traumatic brain injury in the international mission on prognosis and clinical trial design in traumatic brain injury (IMPACT) study. Neurosurgery 2011;68:601-7; discussion 607-8. [PubMed]
  9. Lingsma HF, Roozenbeek B, Perel P, et al. Between-centre differences and treatment effects in randomized controlled trials: a case study in traumatic brain injury. Trials 2011;12:201. [PubMed]
  10. Saatman KE, Duhaime AC, Bullock R, et al. Classification of traumatic brain injury for targeted therapies. J Neurotrauma 2008;25:719-38. [PubMed]
  11. Bell MJ, Adelson PD, Hutchison JS, et al. Differences in medical therapy goals for children with severe traumatic brain injury-an international study. Pediatr Crit Care Med 2013;14:811-8. [PubMed]
  12. Rivara FP, Koepsell TD, Wang J, et al. Incidence of disability among children 12 months after traumatic brain injury. Am J Public Health 2012;102:2074-9. [PubMed]
  13. Adelson PD, Pineda J, Bell MJ, et al. Common data elements for pediatric traumatic brain injury: recommendations from the working group on demographics and clinical assessment. J Neurotrauma 2012;29:639-53. [PubMed]
  14. Berger RP, Beers SR, Papa L, et al. Common data elements for pediatric traumatic brain injury: recommendations from the biospecimens and biomarkers workgroup. J Neurotrauma 2012;29:672-7. [PubMed]
  15. Duhaime AC, Holshouser B, Hunter JV, et al. Common data elements for neuroimaging of traumatic brain injury: pediatric considerations. J Neurotrauma 2012;29:629-33. [PubMed]
  16. Miller AC, Odenkirchen J, Duhaime AC, et al. Common data elements for research on traumatic brain injury: pediatric considerations. J Neurotrauma 2012;29:634-8. [PubMed]
  17. McCauley SR, Wilde EA, Anderson VA, et al. Recommendations for the use of common outcome measures in pediatric traumatic brain injury research. J Neurotrauma 2012;29:678-705. [PubMed]
  18. Mathias JL, Wheaton P. Changes in attention and information-processing speed following severe traumatic brain injury: a meta-analytic review. Neuropsychology 2007;21:212-23. [PubMed]
  19. Aubron C, Cheng AC, Pilcher D, et al. Factors associated with outcomes of patients on extracorporeal membrane oxygenation support: a 5-year cohort study. Crit Care 2013;17:R73. [PubMed]
  20. Ginstfeldt T, Emanuelson I. An overview of attention deficits after paediatric traumatic brain injury. Brain Inj 2010;24:1123-34. [PubMed]
  21. Königs M, Heij HA, van der Sluijs JA, et al. Pediatric Traumatic Brain Injury and Attention Deficit. Pediatrics 2015;136:534-41. [PubMed]
  22. Vos PE, Battistin L, Birbamer G, et al. EFNS guideline on mild traumatic brain injury: report of an EFNS task force. Eur J Neurol 2002;9:207-19. [PubMed]
Cite this article as: Horvat CM, Bell MJ. Bringing attention into higher focus within the traumatic brain injury research agenda. Transl Pediatr 2015;4(4):320-322. doi: 10.3978/j.issn.2224-4336.2015.10.05

Download Citation